Personal tools
News from ICTP 103 - Features - Hear Here
Marcelo Magnasco, who recently joined ICTP's condensed matter physics group, has spent the past decade exploring biological and physical phenomena behind our sense of hearing.
Hear Here
When we hear a songbird sing,
the melodious sounds often generate warm gentle emotions. Sweet-sounding
sounds have the ability to do that to us.
However, when a songbird hears another songbird sing, the emotions
and reactions are often much more significant.
"Song for a songbird is a matter of life and death. The harmonious
sounds that they make are used, for example, to signal territorial
rights and sexual intentions," says Marcelo Magnasco, who
joined ICTP's condensed matter physics group last August after
spending the previous decade with Rockefeller University in New
York City. "Male songbirds often sing continuously for two
or three hours before mating to show their potential partner their
fitness for fathering birdlings."
Despite vast differences in purpose and reaction, hearing among
songbirds and humans have this much in common: Both must somehow
transform sound waves into nerve signals that can be received
and interpreted by the brain.
Marcelo Magnasco
"Hearing," explains Magnasco, "is in many ways
the least well understood of our senses. We can produce technological
analogues of our sense of sight in cameras, and of our sense of
smell in detectors. But, except for implants that operate only
at marginal efficiency, we have yet to produce a technological
analogue for our hearing organ--the cochlea."
Magnasco's research, which he has conducted in partnership with
biologists and physicists (both theorists and experimentalists),
has sought to expand our understanding of how organisms hear what
they hear and respond to those sounds in particular and predictable
ways.
"Only a very few organisms in addition to humans have the
ability to learn and replicate 'meaningful' vocalised sounds,"
notes Magnasco. "Whales, dolphins, and songbirds such as
canaries, hummingbirds and zebra finches are among those in this
select group."
He and his colleagues chose to study songbirds for obvious reasons.
"Transporting whales into our laboratory to examine their
brain's neuron-firing patterns and the biological and physical
dynamics that drive their sense of hearing," Magnasco wryly
notes, "would be a difficult task."
One research project conducted by Magnasco and his colleagues
has shown that the wide range of tonal sounds generated by songbirds
is due to a complex series of behavioural and physiological responses
that begin with a songbird's ability to alter the pressure and
velocity of the air passing through its vocal organ, the syrinx.
The syrinx, in turn, channels the air to the songbird's two bronchial
passages just where these passages meet the windpipe. The labia
or tissue flaps that lie between the bronchial passages and windpipe
vibrate in response to the air current. A diverse set of melodious
tunes is created by the joint action of the pressure created in
the syrinx and the stiffness of the labia. Think of the syrinx
as the body of a clarinet and the labia as the reed. By manipulating
the two, a songbird can croon a wide variety of songs.
In another research project, Magnasco and a group of researchers
investigated how songbirds can distinguish between sounds that
have a similar pitch and tone but originate from different sources--for
example, the same tune made by a bird and imitated electronically.
"When songbirds hear another songbird sing, our laboratory
research reveals intense neural firings and brainwave activity
in specific locations. When the sound is reproduced electronically,
no neural firing takes place in the same locations."
Clearly, songbirds know the difference. Or, more accurately, songbirds
recognise and respond to a 'real' song sung by a 'real' songbird
but do not respond to the same song when it is 'artificially'
replicated. As science writer Henry Gee commented in Nature,
these research findings raise intriguing questions about whether
what we hear is actually taking place or is solely a reflection
of what we are genetically predisposed and trained to hear.
But the most noteworthy research Magnasco has been involved in--research
that has attracted widespread attention in both the scientific
and popular press--has challenged some basic assumptions concerning
the dynamics behind our sense of hearing.
This research has drawn not only on laboratory studies examining
brain wave behaviour in songbirds, but also on a deep understanding
of theoretical physics applied to studies examining how a mammal's
spiral-like hearing organ--the cochlea--may function.
Historically, scientists believed that the cochlea passively absorbed
sound waves, transformed the waves into nerve impulses, and then
electronically transmitted the impulses to the brain in a step-by-step
linear process.
Magnasco, however, has been involved in a series of research initiatives
showing that the cochlea doesn't just passively accept and then
transmit sound waves to the brain, but actually modulates the
waves much like a public address system mixes together the original
sound and its feedback. Or, to change the metaphor, much like
a turbo engine uses the pressure created by the flow of its exhaust
gases to compress the gasoline/oxygen mix.
"The result," Magnasco explains, "is that faint
sounds detected by the cochlea are amplified, which may explain
why mammals, among all organisms, have the capability to hear
faint tones. Conversely, when the sound waves reach a high screeching
pitch, the cochlea has the ability to narrow the sound waves into
a reasonable range that can be tolerated and interpreted by the
brain."
Magnasco's research has continually blurred the boundaries between
disciplines, particularly between physics and biology. Trained
as a theoretical condensed matter physicist at the University
of Chicago, USA, his knowledge and understanding of biology and
neuroscience has been acquired largely through intricate laboratory
experiments and studies involving songbird brain tissue to determine
how a songbird's brain reacts to acoustic stimuli. It has been
a form of on-the-job training that has allowed Magnasco to cross
disciplines without being labelled a renegade by physicists or
an intruder by biologists.
Magnasco realises that the vastly different mindsets that have
shaped the study of physics and biology have kept the two disciplines
far apart. But several recent developments may now be driving
the two closer together, particularly in the study of such phenomena
as the dynamics of hearing, which involves a deep understanding
of many different fields of inquiry.
"First," Magnasco notes, "the sheer volume of data
and information related to biological phenomena, made possible
by electronic data collection, renders it useful--and perhaps
imperative--to develop overarching principles and perhaps models
that can help provide a framework for deciphering information
that may otherwise become overwhelming. Physicists, who are trained
to develop and use theories and models, could assist biologists
in making more effective use of the information they acquire and
in uncovering connections that may be overlooked."
"Second," Magnasco observes, "physicists have shown
an increasing interest in applying their skills in areas beyond
the conventional boundaries of their discipline. And third, both
biologists and physicists (and, for that matter, chemists and
neuroscientists) increasingly recognise that the most advanced
research in their fields is taking place not within but across
disciplines."
In fact, teams of researchers trained in a wide range of fields
are opening new scientific frontiers and pathways of understanding.
That is exactly the strategy that has been applied by the diverse
teams of researchers that Magnasco has worked with in studying
the complex dynamics associated with the sense of hearing. If
the scientific community's enthusiastic response to this research
is any indication, then people are certainly listening.